36 found
Order:
  1.  84
    What is embodiment? A psychometric approach.Matthew R. Longo, Friederike Schüür, Marjolein P. M. Kammers, Manos Tsakiris & Patrick Haggard - 2008 - Cognition 107 (3):978-998.
    No categories
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   112 citations  
  2.  25
    Categorical perception of tactile distance.Frances Le Cornu Knight, Matthew R. Longo & Andrew J. Bremner - 2014 - Cognition 131 (2):254-262.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  3.  50
    Where exactly am I? Self-location judgements distribute between head and torso.Adrian J. T. Alsmith & Matthew R. Longo - 2014 - Consciousness and Cognition 24:70-74.
  4.  38
    Near space and its relation to claustrophobic fear.Stella F. Lourenco, Matthew R. Longo & Thanujeni Pathman - 2011 - Cognition 119 (3):448-453.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  5.  19
    Seeing the body distorts tactile size perception.Matthew R. Longo & Renata Sadibolova - 2013 - Cognition 126 (3):475-481.
  6.  19
    Mental body representations retain homuncular shape distortions: Evidence from Weber’s illusion.Luke E. Miller, Matthew R. Longo & Ayse P. Saygin - 2016 - Consciousness and Cognition 40:17-25.
  7.  28
    Dissociating contributions of head and torso to spatial reference frames: The misalignment paradigm.Adrian J. T. Alsmith, Elisa R. Ferrè & Matthew R. Longo - 2017 - Consciousness and Cognition 53:105-114.
  8.  16
    Intuitive anatomy: Distortions of conceptual knowledge of hand structure.Matthew R. Longo - 2015 - Cognition 142 (C):230-235.
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  9.  16
    Perceptual and Conceptual Distortions of Implicit Hand Maps.Matthew R. Longo, Stefania Mattioni & Nataşa Ganea - 2015 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9.
  10.  27
    The plasticity of near space: Evidence for contraction.Stella F. Lourenco & Matthew R. Longo - 2009 - Cognition 112 (3):451-456.
    No categories
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  11.  12
    A three-dimensional spatial characterization of the crossed-hands deficit.Elena Azañón, Kim Mihaljevic & Matthew R. Longo - 2016 - Cognition 157 (C):289-295.
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  12.  11
    The long sixth finger illusion: The representation of the supernumerary finger is not a copy and can be felt with varying lengths.Denise Cadete & Matthew R. Longo - 2022 - Cognition 218 (C):104948.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  13. Origins and development of generalized magnitude representation.Stella F. Lourenco & Matthew R. Longo - 2011 - In Stanislas Dehaene & Elizabeth Brannon (eds.), Space, Time and Number in the Brain. Oxford University Press. pp. 225--244.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  14.  20
    Visual illusion of tool use recalibrates tactile perception.Luke E. Miller, Matthew R. Longo & Ayse P. Saygin - 2017 - Cognition 162 (C):32-40.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  15.  44
    Tool use induces complex and flexible plasticity of human body representations.Matthew R. Longo & Andrea Serino - 2012 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 35 (4):229 - 230.
    Plasticity of body representation fundamentally underpins human tool use. Recent studies have demonstrated remarkably complex plasticity of body representation in humans, showing that such plasticity (1) occurs flexibly across multiple time scales and (2) involves multiple body representations responding differently to tool use. Such findings reveal remarkable sophistication of body plasticity in humans, suggesting that Vaesen may overestimate the similarity of such mechanisms in humans and non-human primates.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  16.  18
    Reconstructing neural representations of tactile space.Luigi Tamè, Raffaele Tucciarelli, Renata Sadibolova, Martin I. Sereno & Matthew R. Longo - 2021 - NeuroImage 229.
    Psychophysical experiments have demonstrated large and highly systematic perceptual distortions of tactile space. Such a space can be referred to our experience of the spatial organisation of objects, at representational level, through touch, in analogy with the familiar concept of visual space. We investigated the neural basis of tactile space by analysing activity patterns induced by tactile stimulation of nine points on a 3 × 3 square grid on the hand dorsum using functional magnetic resonance imaging. We used a searchlight (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  17.  94
    Explicit and Implicit Own's Body and Space Perception in Painful Musculoskeletal Disorders and Rheumatic Diseases: A Systematic Scoping Review.Antonello Viceconti, Eleonora Maria Camerone, Deborah Luzzi, Debora Pentassuglia, Matteo Pardini, Diego Ristori, Giacomo Rossettini, Alberto Gallace, Matthew R. Longo & Marco Testa - 2020 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 14.
  18.  8
    Expansion of Perceptual Body Maps Near – But Not Across – The Wrist.Matthew R. Longo - 2017 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 11.
  19.  13
    Mapping visual spatial prototypes: Multiple reference frames shape visual memory.Elena Azañón, Raffaele Tucciarelli, Metodi Siromahov, Elena Amoruso & Matthew R. Longo - 2020 - Cognition 198 (C):104199.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  20.  22
    Shared contributions of the head and torso to spatial reference frames across spatial judgments.Matthew R. Longo, Sampath S. Rajapakse, Adrian J. T. Alsmith & Elisa R. Ferrè - 2020 - Cognition 204 (C):104349.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  21.  36
    Posture modulates implicit hand maps.Matthew R. Longo - 2015 - Consciousness and Cognition 36:96-102.
  22.  91
    The person in the mirror: Using the enfacement illusion to investigate the experiential structure of self-identification.Ana Tajadura-Jiménez, Matthew R. Longo, Rosie Coleman & Manos Tsakiris - 2012 - Consciousness and Cognition 21 (4):1725-1738.
    How do we acquire a mental representation of our own face? Recently, synchronous, but not asynchronous, interpersonal multisensory stimulation between one’s own and another person’s face has been used to evoke changes in self-identification. We investigated the conscious experience of these changes with principal component analyses that revealed that while the conscious experience during synchronous IMS focused on resemblance and similarity with the other’s face, during asynchronous IMS it focused on multisensory stimulation. Analyses of the identified common factor structure revealed (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   22 citations  
  23.  15
    A Conceptual Model of Tactile Processing across Body Features of Size, Shape, Side, and Spatial Location.Luigi Tamè, Elena Azañón & Matthew R. Longo - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  24.  10
    Opposite size illusions for inverted faces and letters.Eamonn Walsh, Carolina Moreira & Matthew R. Longo - 2024 - Cognition 245 (C):105733.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  25.  23
    Emotion is perceived accurately from isolated body parts, especially hands.Ellen Blythe, Lúcia Garrido & Matthew R. Longo - 2023 - Cognition 230 (C):105260.
  26.  14
    Body Size Adaptation Alters Perception of Test Stimuli, Not Internal Body Image.Klaudia B. Ambroziak, Elena Azañón & Matthew R. Longo - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  27.  19
    Conceptual distortions of hand structure are robust to changes in stimulus information.Klaudia B. Ambroziak, Luigi Tamè & Matthew R. Longo - 2016 - Consciousness and Cognition 61:107-116.
    Hands are commonly held up as an exemplar of well-known, familiar objects. However, conceptual knowledge of the hand has been found to show highly stereotyped distortions. Specifically, people judge their knuckles as farther forward in the hand than they actually are. The cause of this distal bias remains unclear. In Experiment 1, we tested whether both visual and tactile information contribute to the distortion. Participants judged the location of their knuckles by pointing to the location on their palm directly opposite (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  28.  17
    Eating and body image: Does food insecurity make us feel thinner?Klaudia B. Ambroziak, Elena Azañón & Matthew R. Longo - 2017 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 40.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  29.  16
    Curved sixth fingers: Flexible representation of the shape of supernumerary body parts.Denise Cadete, Adrian J. T. Alsmith & Matthew R. Longo - 2022 - Consciousness and Cognition 105 (C):103413.
  30.  3
    Tactile interactions in the path of tactile apparent motion.Souta Hidaka, Luigi Tamè & Matthew R. Longo - 2021 - Cognition 209 (C):104569.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  31.  29
    Automaticity and inhibition in action planning.Matthew R. Longo & Bennett I. Bertenthal - 2004 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27 (1):44-45.
    We question the generalizability of Glover's model because it fails to distinguish between different forms of planning. The highly controlled experimental situations on which this model is based, do not reflect some important factors that contribute to planning. We discuss several classes of action that seem to imply distinct planning mechanisms, questioning Glover's postulation of a single “planning system.”.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  32.  37
    Flexibility and development of mirroring mechanisms.Matthew R. Longo & Bennett I. Bertenthal - 2008 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31 (1):31-31.
    The empirical support for the shared circuits model (SCM) is mixed. We review recent results from our own lab and others supporting a central claim of SCM that mirroring occurs at multiple levels of representation. By contrast, the model is silent as to why human infants are capable of showing imitative behaviours mediated by a mirror system. This limitation is a problem with formal models that address neither the neural correlates nor the behavioural evidence directly.
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  33.  39
    Merging second-person and first-person neuroscience.Matthew R. Longo & Manos Tsakiris - 2013 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36 (4):429-430.
    Schilbach et al. contrast second-person and third-person approaches to social neuroscience. We discuss relations between second-person and first-person approaches, arguing that they cannot be studied in isolation. Contingency is central for converging first- and second-person approaches. Studies of embodiment show how contingencies scaffold first-person perspective and how the transition from a third- to a second-person perspective fundamentally involves first-person contributions.
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  34.  10
    Mind the Gap: The Effects of Temporal and Spatial Separation in Localization of Dual Touches on the Hand.Renata Sadibolova, Luigi Tamè, Eamonn Walsh & Matthew R. Longo - 2018 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 12.
  35.  3
    Neural correlates of distorted body representations underlying tactile distance perception.Luigi Tamè, Raffaele Tucciarelli, Renata Sadibolova, Martin I. Sereno & Matthew R. Longo - unknown
    Tactile distance perception is believed to require that immediate afferent signals be referenced to a stored representation of body size and shape (the body model). For this ability, recent studies have reported that the stored body representations involved are highly distorted, at least in the case of the hand, with the hand dorsum represented as wider and squatter than it actually is. Here, we aim to define the neural basis of this phenomenon. In a behavioural experiment participants estimated the distance (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  36.  2
    Reconstruction of the neural representations of the tactile space.Luigi Tamè, Raffaele Tucciarelli, Renata Sadibolova, Martin I. Sereno & Matthew R. Longo - unknown
    We examined the neural basis of tactile distance perception by analyzing activity patterns induced by tactile stimulation of nine points on a 3 x 3 square grid on the hand dorsum using functional magnetic resonance (fMRI). We used a searchlight approach within pre-defined regions of interests (ROIs) to compute the pairwise Euclidean distances between the activity patterns elicited by tactile stimulation. Then, we used multidimensional scaling (MDS) to reconstruct skin space at the neural level and compare it with skin space (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark